Neuroscience
Walking cane reveals dramatic sensory re-mapping by the brain
There is a force-field like zone around our bodies, known as peripersonal space, in which our brains integrate information across the senses. For example, we have individual neurons that respond when we sense touch and sound occurring in the same place, such as at or near our hand, at the same time. If you could record the activity of all the relevant individual neurons, you could plot an imaginary border around our bodies within which this sound-touch integration occurs.
Now Andrea Serino and colleagues have shown there is dramatic flexibility in where this sensory integration zone begins and ends. Recording single neurons in healthy participants would be a little intrusive, so the researchers instead took advantage of the fact that where tactile and auditory information is integrated, participants will be quicker to respond to a tactile stimulus when it is accompanied by a sound.
Sixteen sighted participants were blindfolded and held a cane. At first, as expected, they were quicker to respond to a mild electric shock on their hand when that shock was accompanied by a sound located at their hand rather than by a sound at the end of the cane (or no sound at all).
But then they trained for ten minutes with the cane, practising locating objects blindfolded. After the training, the participants were just as quick to respond to a shock on their hand whether it was accompanied by a sound located at their hand, or at the end of the cane - in both cases the accompanying sound speeded their reaction compared with when there was no sound.
This shows there had a been a dramatic remapping of the sound-integration boundary – participants were now integrating sound information at the end of the cane with touch information at their hand. However, by the next day, without any further practice, the boundary had returned to near space and the reaction time advantage was once again only conferred by sounds near their hand.
More extreme, longer-term re-mapping can also occur. The researchers also tested eight blind participants who routinely used a cane. Remarkably, among these participants, touch stimulation at the hand was actually integrated more with sound at the end of the cane than with sound located near the hand.
___________________________________
Serino, A., Bassolino, M., Farne, A. & Ladavas, E. (2007). Extended multisensory space in blind cane users. Psychological Science, 18, 642-648.
Post written by Christian Jarrett (@psych_writer) for the BPS Research Digest.
-
Space Is Compressed By A Fast Turn Of Your Head
The raw immediacy of our waking lives leaves us feeling as though our five senses give us a true, undistorted perception of the world. But a catalogue of psychology experiments has shown this sense of experiencing the world "as it is" couldn't be...
-
Getting In Touch With Our Shadows
The sight of our own shadows can affect our sense of touch. That's according to Francesco Pavani and Giovanni Galfano who have shown that seeing the shadow of a part of your body automatically directs your tactile attention to that body part. Forty-two...
-
Ninety Minutes Blindfolded Enhances Your Hearing
In blind people, the part of the brain usually used for vision can be commandeered by other senses, resulting in improved hearing and touch. It’s an amazing testament to the brain’s ability to adapt. But now, Jorg Lewald reports that prolonged blindness...
-
Forget Stroop, Here's The Snarc
You’ve probably heard of the Stroop effect (if not, see here), now let me introduce you to the SNARC. The Spatial Numerical Association of Response Codes (SNARC) effect is the observation that people are faster to make a judgment about a number if the...
-
Bzzzpeek!
BzzzPeek -- international onomatopoeia using sound recordings from native speakers imitating the sounds of animals [flash & sound required]...
Neuroscience